The Foundational Research Behind MathFactLab

Please consider the text below to be a work in progress.

MathFactLab was developed to provide students with a research-backed approach to online math fact fluency practice. While most math fact websites are memorization-based, MathFactLab students develop fact fluency by doing what research recommends: practicing with multiple representations and strategies - first working with the foundational facts, and subsequently using these to construct the derived facts.

The guiding principles by which MathFactLab was designed were all informed by current math educational research. Each of these principles is covered below:

There are three phases of math fact fluency development.

According to Baroody, there are three phases of math fact learning: (1) Counting strategies, in which the counting of objects is a necessary step to finding an answer; (2) Reasoning strategies, where logic, deduction, and the combination of known facts leads to the solution of an unknown fact; (3) Mastery, in which answers are produced efficiently and accurately (Baroody, 2006).

The initial phase should occur in the classroom, utilizing manipulatives under the guidance of a teacher. Developing strategies, the second of these phases, should also first be explored under the guidance of a teacher, whether it be with ten frames, number lines, rekenreks, area models, dice or other tools. Van de Walle writes, 'Phase 2 is often under-emphasized or neglected entirely, yet it is an essential bridge between inefficient counting strategies and mastery (2019).

When students are at the point where they can begin to apply strategies using manipulatives to solve fact problems more efficiently than with counting, they are ready to begin practicing with MathFactLab. At any level in the program, where a specific set of fact families are practiced, the program serves as a bridge, guiding students through Stage 2 until they achieve fluent Stage-3 mastery. Afterward, they advance to the next level in the program and repeat the process.

Memorization and drill are ineffective ways to teach math facts.

As Boaler puts it, 'Mathematics facts are important but the memorization of math facts through times table repetition, practice and timed testing is unnecessary and damaging' (2015). Memorization skips over Stage 2 in the fact-learning progression. Students are introduced to concepts and then are expected to memorize the facts. As Van de Walle put it, 'There is strong evidence that this method simply does not work' (2019). According to Baroody, this leads to 'Inefficiency', 'Inappropriate applications', and 'Inflexibility' (2006). A memorization approach to math fact learning also inhibits the development of number sense (Boaler, 2015).

Strategy-based math fact instruction works.

There is ample evidence to show that the teaching and practicing of strategies to solve math facts is effective (Delazur, 2005; National Research Council, 2001; Cook and Dossey, 1982; Heege, 1985; Kami, 1994). Van de Walle writes, 'Research supports the use of explicit strategy instruction as effective in helping all students learn (and remember) their basic facts (e.g., Baroody, et al., 2009; Baroody, et al., 2016; Thornton, 1978; Fuson, 1992; Rathmell, 1978; Thornton & Toohey, 1984)' (2019).

Explicit strategy instruction allows students to solve facts efficiently while avoiding the pitfalls of counting or memorization. As O'Connell and San Giovanni put it, 'Teaching math fact strategies focuses attention on number sense, operations, patterns, properties, and other critical number concepts. These big ideas related to numbers provide a strong foundation for the strategic reasoning that supports mastering basic math facts' (2015).

Conceptual knowledge of the four basic operations and their relationships builds an 'organizing framework for storing arithmetic facts' (Davenport, et al., 2019). 'Children who commit the facts to memory easily are able to do so because they have constructed relationships among them and between addition and subtraction [or multiplication and division] in general, and they use these relationships as shortcuts' (Fosnot & Dolk, 2001).

MathFactLab students work with models that they are familiar with from the classroom and that build conceptual understanding: ten frames, beads, number lines, area models, dice, and bar diagrams. With these models, students practice strategies that help them construct new math fact knowledge by tapping prior knowledge.  For example, students practice their near doubles (6+7, 9+8) using ten frames by adding one to the doubles facts that they already know (6+6 or 8+8).  In multiplication, for example, students learn how to solve 6x4 by adding two groups, each composed of 3 fours represented with dice.

Students should first learn the foundational facts and use these to construct the derived facts.

MathFactLab is organized into stages, each broken down into multiple levels. The initial stage, 'Basic Part I' - whether in our addition/subtraction or our multiplication/division learning modes - focuses on developing fluency with the foundational facts. 'Basic Part 2' teaches strategies that use the foundational facts to build the derived facts.

The levels within each stage focus on a group of fact families that can all be practiced using the same set of strategies. The levels are ordered so that easier strategies are introduced first and can be generally applied to larger sets of fact families. This leaves fewer fact families to be learned using the more difficult strategies (Thornton, 1978; Kling & Bay-Williams, 2015). For example, Level A's simple plus-one strategy covers eleven fact families while the more challenging strategies of Level K(+7) only need to be applied to two. The order of these levels and the strategies covered at each level follow the guidance of current research (Watanabe, 2003; Van de Walle, et al., 2019; Bay-Williams & Kling, 2019; O'Connell & SanGiovanni, 2011; Davenport, et al., 2019).

Fact practice should enhance and be grounded in number sense.

Boaler defines number sense as a 'deep understanding of numbers and the ways they relate to each other.' When students have number sense, they can 'use numbers flexibly' (2015). By helping students to discover multiple routes to solving math facts, MathFactLab helps students become flexible mathematicians, leading them to current and future success in mathematics.


Bibliography

Anghileri, J., Beishuizen, M., & Van Putten, K. (2002). From informal strategies to structured procedures: Mind the gap!. Educational Studies in Mathematics49, 149-170.

Baroody, Arthur J., David J. Purpura, Michael D. Eiland, and Erin E. Reid. "Fostering First Graders' Fluency With Basic Subtraction and Larger Addition Combinations Via Computer-Assisted Instruction." Cognition and Instruction 32.2 (2014): 159-97.

Baroody, Arthur J. "An Evaluation of Evidence Supporting Fact-retrieval Models." Learning and Individual Differences 6.1 (1994): 1-36.

Baroody, Arthur J. "Mastery of Basic Number Combinations: Internalization of Relationships or Facts?" Journal for Research in Mathematics Education 16.2 (1985): 83-98.

Baroody, Arthur J, David J Purpura, Michael D Eiland, Erin E Reid, and Veena Paliwal. "Does Fostering Reasoning Strategies for Relatively Difficult Basic Combinations Promote Transfer by K-3 Students?" Journal of Educational Psychology 108.4 (2016): 576-91.

Baroody, A. J. (2006, August). Why Children Have Difficulties Mastering the Basic Number Combinations and How to Help Them. Teaching Children Mathematics, 13(1), 22–31.

Baroody, Arthur J., Neet Priya Bajwa, and Michael Eiland. "Why Can't Johnny Remember the Basic Facts?" Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews 15.1 (2009): 69-79.

Bay-Williams, J. M., & Kling, G. (2019). Math fact fluency: 60+ games and assessment tools to support learning and retention. ASCD.

Boaler, Jo. "Research Suggests That Timed Tests Cause Math Anxiety." Teaching Children Mathematics 20.8 (2014): 469-74.

Boaler, J. (2015) Fluency Without Fear: Research Evidence on the Best Ways to Learn Math Facts. https://www.youcubed.org/wp-content/uploads/2017/09/Fluency-Without-Fear-1.28.15.pdf

Brendefur, J., Strother, S., Thiede, K., & Appleton, S. (2015). Developing multiplication fact fluency. Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal.

Brownell, W. A., & Chazal, C. B. (1935). The effects of premature drill in third-grade arithmetic. The Journal of Educational Research29(1), 17-28.

Bruner, J.S. (1964). The Course of Cognitive Growth, American Psychologist 19(1):1-15

Carpenter, Thomas P., and James M. Moser. "The Acquisition of Addition and Subtraction Concepts in Grades One through Three." Journal for Research in Mathematics Education 15.3 (1984): 179-202.

Davenport, L. R., Henry, C. S., Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2019, January 1). No More Math Fact Frenzy. Heinemann Educational Books.

Delazer, M., A. Ischebeck, F. Domahs, L. Zamarian, F. Koppelstaetter, C.M. Siedentopf, L. Kaufmann, T. Benke, and S. Felber. "Learning by Strategies and Learning by Drill—evidence from an FMRI Study." NeuroImage (Orlando, Fla.) 25.3 (2005): 838-49. Web.

Deunk, M. I., Smale-Jacobse, A. E., de Boer, H., Doolaard, S., & Bosker, R. J. (2018). Effective differentiation practices: A systematic review and meta-analysis of studies on the cognitive effects of differentiation practices in primary education. Educational Research Review24, 31-54.

Donovan, J. J., & Radosevich, D. J. (1999). A meta-analytic review of the distribution of practice effect: Now you see it, now you don't. Journal of Applied Psychology84(5), 795.

Dowker, A. (2014). Young children's use of derived fact strategies for addition and subtraction. Frontiers in human neuroscience7, 924.

Fosnot, C. T., & Dolk, M. (2001). Young Mathematicians at Work: Constructing Number Sense, Addition, and Subtraction. Heinemann, 88 Post Road West, PO Box 5007, Westport, CT 06881.

Heege, Hans Ter. "The Acquisition of Basic Multiplication Skills." Educational Studies in Mathematics 16.4 (1985): 375-88.

Henry, V. J., & Brown, R. S. (2008). First-grade basic facts: An investigation into teaching and learning of an accelerated, high-demand memorization standard. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education39(2), 153-183.

Isaacs, Andrew C., and William M. Carroll. "STRATEGIES FOR Basic-Facts Instruction." Teaching Children Mathematics 5.9 (1999): 508-15.

Kilpatrick, Jeremy., Swafford, Jane, Findell, Bradford, and National Research Council . Mathematics Learning Study Committee. Adding It up : Helping Children Learn Mathematics / Mathematics Learning Study Committee, Center for Education, Division of Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education, National Research Council ; Jeremy Kilpatrick, Jane Swafford, and Bradford Findell, Editors. Washington, DC: National Academy, 2001.

Kim, S. H. (2015). A meta-analysis on the effects of the differentiated instruction in mathematics. The Mathematical Education54(4), 335-350.

Kling, Gina, and Jennifer M. Bay-Williams. "Three Steps to Mastering Multiplication Facts." Teaching Children Mathematics 21.9 (2015): 548-59.

Kling, Gina, and Jennifer M Bay-Williams. "Assessing Basic Fact Fluency." Teaching Children Mathematics 20.8 (2014): 488-97.

Mainali, B. (2021). Representation in Teaching and Learning Mathematics. International Journal of Education in Mathematics, Science and Technology9(1), 1-21.

Mulligan, Joanne T., and Michael C. Mitchelmore. "Young Children's Intuitive Models of Multiplication and Division." Journal for Research in Mathematics Education 28.3 (1997): 309-30.

Nelson, Peter M., David C. Parker, and Anne F. Zaslofsky. "The Relative Value of Growth in Math Fact Skills Across Late Elementary and Middle School." Assessment for Effective Intervention 41.3 (2016): 184-92. Web.

O’Connell, S., & SanGiovanni, J. (2015). Mastering the basic math facts in addition and subtraction: Strategies, Activities & interventions to move students beyond memorization. Heinemann.

Paliwal, Veena, and Arthur J. Baroody. "Fostering the Learning of Subtraction Concepts and the Subtraction-as-addition Reasoning Strategy." Early Childhood Research Quarterly 51 (2020): 403-15.

Ramirez, G., Gunderson, E. A., Levine, S. C., & Beilock, S. L. (2013). Math anxiety, working memory, and math achievement in early elementary school. Journal of cognition and development14(2), 187-202.

Sherin, B., & Fuson, K. (2005). Multiplication strategies and the appropriation of computational resources. Journal for research in mathematics education36(4), 347-395.

Solomon, Tracy L, and John Mighton. "Developing Mathematical Fluency: A Strategy to Help Children Learn Their Multiplication Facts." Perspectives on Language and Literacy 43.1 (2017): 31.

Thornton, Carol A. "Emphasizing Thinking Strategies in Basic Fact Instruction." Journal for Research in Mathematics Education 9.3 (1978): 214-27.

Thornton, Carol A. "Solution Strategies: Subtraction Number Facts." Educational Studies in Mathematics 21.3 (1990): 241-63.

Tournaki, Nelly. "The Differential Effects of Teaching Addition Through Strategy Instruction Versus Drill and Practice to Students With and Without Learning Disabilities." Journal of Learning Disabilities 36.5 (2003): 449-58.

Van de Walle, John A., Karen S. Karp, and Jennifer M. Bay-Williams. Elementary and middle school mathematics: Teaching developmentally. Pearson. One Lake Street, Upper Saddle River, New Jersey 07458, 2022.

Watanabe, Tad. "Teaching Multiplication: An Analysis of Elementary School Mathematics Teachers' Manuals from Japan and the United States." The Elementary School Journal 104.2 (2003): 111-25.